Lloyd Morgan's Column
[Return to column index]
Commentary - Mobile phone use and risk of glioma in adults: case-control
study
(Hepworth et al., BMJ 19 January 2006)
In years past, this paper would never have been published because such a low
participation rate of cases and controls would have, by itself, been a cause for
rejection. With only 51% of glioma victims (30% had died by the time they were
contacted) and 45% of the controls (29% refused to participate) participating,
combined with more affluent controls than cases, there is little reason to
believe any of the reported results.[1]
The study would seem to suggest that using a cellphone would protect the
cellphone use from the risk of glioma. If there is no risk of glioma from
cellphone use, then there would be about the same number of odds ratios greater
than one (increased risk) as there would be odds ratios less than one (reduced
risk). Overall this study reports 34 odds ratios greater than one to 8 odds
ratios less than one. This is equivalent to saying if I flip a coin 42 times,
what is the chance that I will get 34 heads and 8 tails. Such a probability can
be calculated. The probability that cellphone use protects the user from glioma
is 99.997%.
There are only two possible conclusions. Either a cellphone use protects the
user from glioma or the study is flawed. You, the reader, must choose between
these two possibilities. There is no other choice possible.
In spite of these incredible flaws, the study did report a 60% increased risk
of glioma for regular cellphone use of 10 years or more on the same side of the
head as the location of the tumor. Another study has found similar results,
though with much higher risk of glioma.[2]
The reference group for this study's findings is "Never/non-regular"
cellphone users. Because this reference group did not exclude the users of
cordless (DECT) phone the reference group cannot be describe as unexposed.
Hardell et al. had reported that cordless phone users who did not use cellphone
are at risk of high-grade astrocytoma (OR=1.9, CI; 1.01-3.5).[3]
Finally, it is important to understand that this study, as is true with every
Interphone study, has received substantial funding from the cellphone industry.
Though the study reports that this funding "ensures complete independence for
the scientific investigators," this is inherently not possible. Researchers
careers are dependent on receiving research grants. Even with isolation of
funding for a specific study from the researchers themselves, the
conflict-of-interest in such funding is not resolved. Because the researchers
know where the funding has come from, the old adage, "Don't bite the hand that
feeds you" becomes the effective psychological reality (whether conscious or
unconscious).
This same conflict-of-interest issue can be seen within the Federal Drug
Administration (FDA) where pharmaceutical companies pay fees for drug approval
isolated from specific research projects. It is quite apparent that the FDA has
come to see the pharmaceutical industry as their customer, not the American
public.
Also to note: just like the pharmaceutical industry-funded studies,
industry-funded cellphone studies will not release their protocols - so that
there is limited assurance both as to the validity of their study procedures and
the steps take to shield research from economic interests.
For example, the Interphone study protocol, a common protocol used by 13
participating countries, is kept "strictly confidential." While there is a
process to maintain the independence of researchers from direct cellphone
industry involvement, it is unclear as to what and how the cellphone industry
was involved in the Interphone study protocol design. Certainly, the
Interphone definition of "regular" cellphone use is set to such a minimal
standard that few could imagine a finding of risk. Regular cellphone use is
defined as cellphone use for at least once a week for six months or more, a year
prior to the study cutoff date.
The important question to ask is, if this were a study of the risk of lung
cancer from smoking would there be a likelihood of finding a risk of lung cancer
from smokers who had smoked at least once a week for 6 months or more, a year
prior to the study cutoff date? And, would there be a finding of risk, if as is
the case in this study for cellphone use, the lifetime years of smoking for 10
years or more included only 3.9% of the smokers in the study?
References
[1] Affluent participating controls are more likely to use cellphone than the
non-participating controls. Such a "selection bias" would result in an
underestimation of the risk of glioma from cellphone use. The sheer proportion
of non-participating controls makes such selection bias highly probable.
[2] Hardell et al., Environmental Research 12 July 2005 reported for wireless
phone use on the same side of the head, the risk of high-grade astrocytoma was
4.2, 3.2 and 4.0 for analog and digital cellphone and cordless phone use,
respectively. The Hepworth et al. paper did not reference this study, even
though it was published some 4 months prior to acceptance of the Hepworth
paper.
[3] Ibid, Table 5.
|