Lloyd Morgan's Column
[Return to column index]
Cellphones and Human Health
Introduction
It is our contention, based solely on our reading of the scientific literature to date, that human exposure to cellphones poses a major health threat. Yet, everywhere we hear sanguine reports of cellphone use. (One of the latest was a cheery story on CBS TV's Good Morning show, April 1st, 2005, regaling, without a mention of potential harm, the "wonderful" new cellphone products being developed by several corporations for 8-11 year olds!) What is to be made of these differing views?
The central thesis of this discussion is that the divergent views on the health effects of cellphone exposure lie not in "the truth" of the scientific findings but rather in the pre-existing interpretive "booby traps" unique to studies involving brain tumors, and indeed unique to studies of cellphone use, especially at this stage of cellphone exposure. To therefore make sense of what can sometimes be seen as dueling studies and conflicting claims, this discussion will try to provide a working understanding of the interpretive "booby traps" which lay in wait. This examination will then be followed by a reinterpretation of the scientific literature in hopes of providing a fresh look-and a more accurate picture of what may befall us with near-universal exposure to cellphones in our future.
Interpretive Booby Traps of Cellphone Studies
1. Difficulty in Interpreting the "Numbers"
Actual harm caused by exposure to an activity/substance is generally measured by counting how much "bad stuff", or cases of harm, is resultant. However, in many circumstances-most especially where the "harm" is manifested as brain tumors-this measurement is not so simple.
One issue in relying uncritically on the number of brain tumors developed after exposure is the rarity of brain tumors. In the broad brush, if there are too few brain tumor cases, then the study will have little statistical power to determine whether, or not, a risk exists.
Rare diseases are difficult for epidemiology, as a relatively large study is required to have any statistical power to resolve an increased risk. Brain tumors, both "benign"[1] and malignant, are indeed a rare disease with an incidence in the United States of 141 cases per million people, per year. If only malignant brain tumors are considered, as is the case in many cellphone studies of brain tumors, then the US incidence is 73 cases per million people, per year.[2] Acoustic neuromas, the most common brain tumor associated with cellphone use is even more rare. The US incidence rate of acoustic neuromas is 54% of all nerve sheath tumors.[3] The incidence rate for acoustic neuromas is a mere 6 per million people per year.[4]
1 - In the context of brain tumors, I use quotes around the term "benign" with irony as the dictionary defines benign as harmless.
2 - 2004-2005, Primary Brain Tumors in the United States, Statistical Report, 1997-2001, Year Data Collected, p. 9; Central Brain Tumor Registry of the United States (www.cbtrus.org), 2004.
3 - 2004-2005, Primary Brain Tumors in the United States, Statistical Report, 1997-2001, Year Data Collected, p. 7; Central Brain Tumor Registry of the United States, 2004.
4 - Calculated result using Table 8, p.32 in 2004-2005, Primary Brain Tumors in the United States, Statistical Report, 1997-2001, Year Data Collected; Central Brain Tumor Registry of the United States, 2004.
Generally, for statistical meaningfulness, it is felt that a sample of no less than 30 subjects is required. Though a given study may select a far larger number than 30, what is important is the number for the sub-groups of interest. For example, Christensen et al. (see Study 8 below) started with 141 cases. After excluding cases for various reasons, there were 106 cases. When they had determined how many cases actually used a cellphone there were 45 cases. When these cases were further divided into years of use, there were 27, 17, and 2 cases for 1-4 years, 5-9 years and >10 years respectively. Not surprisingly, when sub-divided into the years of use, all of the results were so far from statistical significance that there was no meaningful result. Yet Study 8 summarized its findings by reporting, "The results of this ... study ... do not support an association between cell phone use and risk of acoustic neuroma."
So we see from this example, that a negative finding of harm in a scientific study with too small of a sample, must actually be interpreted as meaningless. It tells us nothing. It does not say, "there is no risk" nor does it say, "there is a risk."
2. Cohort vs. Case-Controlled Studies
In interpreting cellphone studies it is not only important to examine the amount of raw numbers, but it is also vital for a meaningful interpretation to understand the context in which these numbers were obtained.
An example of the role that number context plays in cellphone studies can be found in the differences that lay in two epidemiological research models: cohort studies versus case-controlled studies. A cohort study uses a large population (the cohort), which is then used to examine how many cases are observed versus expected to determine whether there is an excess risk or not. For rare diseases such as brain tumors, it is likely that there will be scarcely enough cases to determine if there is a risk or not. As an example of this difficulty in determining risk from a cohort study, it is important to examine the Danish cohort study, discussed later as Study 3. In this study the size of the cohort (Danish cellphone subscribers) is 420,095. However, because of the rareness of brain tumors, they only observed 11 temporal lobe brain tumors compared to 12.8 expected. The result is again so far from statistical significance that no meaningful conclusion can be drawn. Yet it provides a good example of the irrelevance of the results derived from a cohort study approaching a half million people.
Case-controlled studies, on the other hand, are a better model design to study rare diseases. Case-control studies select cases, and then find controls to match each case. The importance of this approach is that the study starts by finding a sufficient number of cases (hopefully) to provide sufficient "statistical power" to determine whether or not there is a risk above a defined level.[5] Each control is commonly matched (a matched case-control study) to each case by various attributes such as age, gender, ethnicity, income and so on. With the selection of the cases and the controls, a common questionnaire is answered by each case and each control.
Questionnaires are not practical for cohort studies because of the size of the cohort, so less is known about the cohort than is known about the cases and controls of a case-control study. The Danish cohort study (Study 3) was accomplished using data linkages between the cellphone company's records and the Danish cancer registry information. No member of the cohort was contacted.
3.Latency and Duration of Exposure
Another interpretive difficulty is assessing the harm within the context of the latency time between exposure to a possible carcinogen and the diagnosis of the tumor. When analyzing a cellphone study it is essential to understand the length of time that a subject has been using a cellphone. A study may have a reasonable number of brain tumor cases, and the context of these cases may make these numbers robust, but when we consider some minimum time of use, the number of brain tumor cases is reduced, often substantially.
With all cancers, there is a significant latency time between exposure to a carcinogen and tumor diagnosis. For all brain tumors (malignant and "benign" combined) the latency time ranges from 20 to 40 years.[6] For the most common "benign" brain tumor, meningioma, the latency time is thought to be between 30 and 40 years.[7,8]
What is alarming is that cellphone studies are reporting much shorter latency times. One cellphone study reports median latency times of 7 years for analog cellphones, 5 years for cordless phones, and 3 years for digital cellphones;[9] another study reports a latency time for acoustic neuromas from cellphone exposure of 10-years or greater.[10] If we believe that average latency times are on the order of several decades, then we can assume that these initial findings of tumors that reflect only about one decade are just the tip of the iceberg. If these early indications of a risk for brain tumors are true, then what will be the increased risk when cellphone exposures have existed for multiple decades?
5 - "Statistical power" is a formal and calculable term. For example, a paper may state, "The statistical power of this study provides an 80% confidence of finding a 2-fold or higher risk, if there is a risk."
6 - "Brain cancer is often described as having a lengthy latency or development period, sometimes in excess of 20 years." Massachusetts Department Of Public Health, Bureau of Environmental Health Assessment, Community Assessment Program; Assessment of Brain and Central Nervous System Cancer Incidence in Needham, MA 1987-1998; December 2002
7 - Sadetzki S, Flint-Richter P, Ben-Tal T, Nass D: Radiation-induced meningioma: a descriptive study of 253 cases. J Neurosurg 97: 1078-1082, 2002
8 - Shintani T, Hayakawa N, Hoshi M, Sumida M, Kurisu K, Oki S, Kodama Y, Kajikawa H, Inai K, Kamada N: High incidence of meningioma among Hiroshima atomic bomb survivors. J Radiat Res (Tokyo) 40: 49-57, 1999
9 - European Journal of Cancer Prevention 2002, 11, 1-10
10 - Epidemiology 2004;15: 653-659
4. Ages of Subjects
It is also important to recognize that the ages of the subjects in cellphone studies warrant interpretive attention. That is, the increased risk of a brain tumor is much higher in younger, than in older people after exposure to a carcinogen. This age-relatedness of increased cancer risks to exposure to carcinogens was first established in the study of exposure to ionizing radiation (as opposed to the non-ionizing radiation of cellphones) in atomic bomb survivors. It is generally accepted that the reason the risk from a carcinogen is higher for younger people is because the young have more cells dividing.
Figure 1 below shows the relative risk of developing cancer for the survivors of the 1945 atomic bombs compared to people not exposed to the bomb.[11] In this example the relative risk is the increased risk cause by the ionizing radiation from the atomic bomb. Thus a relative risk of 1.0 means that the cancer rate would be the same if there were no exposure to the atomic bomb. The graph clearly shows that age at exposure is a strong function of the relative excess rate of solid cancers for ionizing radiation.
While an argument may be made that, lacking parallel studies linking exposure to non-ionizing cellphone radiation with higher carcinogenic risk for younger groups (as we have with ionizing radiation), our fears for risk-association with youth are groundless. However, Hardell et al. (Study 10) does just this: provides us with the first study linking younger age at exposure to increased brain tumor risk from non-ionizing exposures. Its findings show far higher risk for the youngest age group exposed to non-ionizing radiation than for older age groups, the same as we have come to expect with ionizing radiation exposure.
Because the rate of cellular growth is so much higher in children than in younger adult groups any indication of increased risk for younger adult groups suggests that the risk of cellphone use by children may be higher yet. Study 10 should be read as a "canary in the mine" warning. Indeed, scientists and policy makers in the U.K. have been so alarmed at the first studies finding increased harm to youth that an advisory warning has been instituted for cellphone use by children.[12]
11 - Preston et al., Studies of Mortality of Atomic Bomb Survivors. Report 13: Solid Cancer and Noncancer Disease Mortality: 1950-1997; RADIATION RESEARCH 160, 381-407 (2003), Adapted from FIG. 1. Excess cancer rates by calendar periods and age-at-exposure categories: absolute and relative to background rates.
12 - Associated Press report at Forbes
5.Tumor Location Analysis
Another interpretive risk for which we need to be mindful is failure to pay
close attention to both the location of the tumor and the location of the
cellphone exposure in relation to the tumor. Brain tumors that are counted for a
study that are beyond the cellphone's radiation "plume" (e.g., brain stem or
cerebellum tumors) may result in an inaccurate interpretation of the true
dangers, if any, of cellphone use. On the other hand, when the location of
cellphone exposure (i.e., the side of the head where the cellphone is held) is
not indicated, the ability to measure the risk between location of exposure and
location of tumors is unfortunately diluted. Ideally a study should emphasize
temporal lobe and acoustic neuroma tumors occurring on the same side of the head
where the cellphone was predominantly used, since this is where the cellphone
radiation plume is strongest.
6. Differing Wireless Phone Technology: Does It Matter?
Yet another interpretive trap to be wary of is proffered arguments denying
harm based on differing wireless phone technologies. Along these lines it has
lately become fashionable to dismiss any positive finding of harm to the fact
that the analysis finding harm is based on "old" technologies, while pointing to
the fact that the "newer" technologies show less, or no findings of harm.
For example in Studies 5 and 6, an elevated risk of harm is found for users
of analog cellphones (an older technology) but less risk of harm for digital
cordless phones (an intermediate technology) and the least for digital cell
phones (newest technology). The higher risk of harm that is evident for users of
analog phones is brushed away as a non-issue because, "Analog cellphones are not
used any more, anyway". However easy this brush off may be, the facts of the
matter deserve a deeper examination.
Firstly, the differences in technology between analog and digital cellphones
or cordless phones are arbitrary and are based only on differing engineering
terminology—concepts that have not been shown to have a differing biological
basis. These engineering differences between analog and digital cellphones or
cordless phones are changes to modulation techniques (how information is created
by changing the signals), and in changes to carrier frequencies (different
frequencies change "channels" to receive, or send, differing signals).
Secondly, cellphone technology is constantly changing, but the changes
continue to be focused on the engineering changes described above. Therefore,
assuming that modulated radiation may be the factor of harm, then the
"different" exposures provided by each new cellphone technology rather than
being altered, remain constant.
Thirdly, what does change, and is reflected in all cellphone studies (and is
the true relevant factor concerning differing technologies), is the length of
time of use. As discussed in Interpretive Booby Trap 3, "Latency Time", one of
the most important factors in any study concerning exposure to agents that may
cause tumors, is the length of time of that exposure. It makes perfect sense
that analog cellphones (oldest used) show consistently highest risk of harm, as
opposed to digital cordless phones (next oldest, with a risk between analog and
digital cellphones) and last, digital cellphones (newest and showing least
risk): analog cellphones have been used the longest and have been used by more
people the longest. The other two phone technologies studied have a lower
latency time, and have been used by a smaller population. It is therefore clear
that when a study shows findings of harm from exposure to cellphones, regardless
of its technology, old or new, if the technology of greatest length of exposure
shows increased harm—it is cause for great concern. And there is no reason to
believe that as the newer technologies are used for an equivalent period of
time, say 10 years or more, that similar factors of risk will not be seen.
A few more notes concerning the length of use time and cellphone type:
Cordless phone conversations usually last for much longer than do cellphone
conversations. Partially this is a result of the cost of cellphone use time and
partially this is the result of the quality of the connection. And, as far as
the use time for cellphones, we must consider the fact that parents often
provide young children with cordless phones even when they do not allow these
same children to have cellphones. This may be because while there has been a
general public perception that there might be a problem with cellphones,
cordless phones have heretofore been considered safe.
A last point on the significance of differing wireless phone technologies is
that of the difference in "power" between analog, and digital cellphones, and
digital cordless phones. It is true that analog cellphones are almost 10 times
more powerful than digital cellphones or cordless phones (cordless phones
radiate about the same power as digital cellphones). But the extra power is not
the same as extra "dose" with which it is often confused.
The power absorbed by a cellphone user's brain is considered the "dose" and
is measured using a factor called SAR, or Specific Absorption Rate. The units
for SAR are watts per kilogram.[13] In other
words, the power absorbed per weight of brain tissue. We might think that
cellphones radiating the same power (Watts) would have similar SAR values. This
is not the case. SAR values were measured for 16 different digital cellphones,
each radiating the same power. The SAR values ranged from 0.28 W/kg to 1.33
W/kg, a factor of 4.75.[14] We might think that
for a given phone's SAR value that the specific SAR we receive would be the
same. But this is not the case either. The way someone holds the cellphone is
also important: "... when the phone is slightly tilted towards the head ... the
value can go from 0.2 to 3.5 W/kg"[15] - a
factor of 17.5. Therefore, to imbue undo significance to the amount of power
absorbed by our brain (AKA SAR) when using a cellphone is missing the bigger
picture. SAR varies to such an extent that the specific power that a phone
radiates becomes a secondary if not tertiary factor. In fact, when examining the
aggregate power differences between the analog and digital cellphone and the
different ways of holding the cellphone, the only conclusion that can be drawn
is that of "a rather large uncertainty in the actual SAR determination for a
specific situation, with a factor of 100 from the nearness to the base station
and at least a factor of 10-50 depending on make and model and personal style of
use."[16]
7. Harm Standards: Do They Make Sense?[17,18]
Another interpretive dilemma regarding cellphone studies concerns the very
standard that the cellphone scientific community demands for a finding of harm.
This scientific community actually demands the meeting of a two-tier test as the
level of certainty required for causal evidence linkage. The level of certainty
required for this cumulative test is 95%-99% - a nearly impossible scientific
hoop to jump through.
13 - Interestingly, the ionizing radiation
dose is SA, or Specific Absorption. SAR is just the SA received per second (the
Rate).
14 - Kuster, N. (1997), Swiss Tests Show Wide
Variation in Radiation Exposures from Cell Phones; Microwave News,
November/December 1997
15 - Hansson-Mild et al, "Exposure" and "Dose"
in Mobile Phone Health Studies; Mobile Communication and Health, Medical,
Biological and Social Problems, Moscow, Sept. 2004
16 - Ibid
17 - This discussion is based in part on a
presentation by Cindy Sage during the Bioelectromagnetics Society's meeting,
Washington DC, June 2004
18 - Part of this discussion, and the next
section (The Role of "Industry") are also based on comments by Dr. Michael Kundi
in the March/April 2004 Bioelectromagnetics Newsletter
The first requirement that must be met is a finding of "95% confidence level"
for each individual scientific study before a claim of "concern" of harm will be
accepted. The general meaning of "95% confidence level" is that there is only a
5% chance that a given finding of risk happens by chance; or, conversely, that
there is a 95% chance of the finding of risk being a true result.
To further compound the difficulty of producing an accepted finding of harm,
scientists studying the biological effects of cellphone exposure must satisfy
the second-tier requirements that include, but are not limited to, the
following: multiple replications of each finding at the cellular (in-vitro), the
animal (in-vivo), and human epidemiological levels (and again, all these studies
must meet the 95% confidence level). Additionally, these combined results must
then reach a consensus finding of harm by both the independent and the cellphone
industry components of the scientific community (see discussion in The Role of
"Industry", below).
As can be seen, these multiple standards require not only virtual certainty
in a complex world where not much can be found to be absolutely certain, but
also endless funding and time.
To fully comprehend the scientific trap that we have created by using this
impossible set of standards it is instructive to examine the case of tobacco. It
is now readily accepted by both the lay and scientific communities that tobacco
is carcinogenic and therefore "causes" cancer. Yet scientists conducted decades
of animal studies without a single "positive" finding. Indeed, today the
evidence of harm for tobacco could not possibly reach the standard for concern
for which cellphones are now held. However, with only about 10% of the smoking
population being diagnosed with lung cancer, we would be negligent, indeed, to
not at least call for a cautious use of tobacco.
How do other communities that shape public policy verify that harm has
occurred?
American Legal System
For almost all civil cases, the standard required for evidentiary findings of
proof in the legal system is preponderance of the evidence; in other
words, is the "existence of a particular fact more probable than its
nonexistence".[19] This standard of proof,
then, really only requires the barest tipping point beyond equipoise, or,
approximately 51%.
The legal system also has two other standards: clear and convincing
proof (which asks for a "substantially greater degree of belief concerning
the existence of a fact);[20] and for criminal cases, beyond a reasonable
doubt (which calls for proof that is "inconsistent with any other rational
conclusions").[21,22]
Although, the degree of certainty that is required for these two stricter legal
standards is not usually expressed as exact numerical percentages, a reasonable
guess could interpret these standards as somewhere between 70% and 80% - a
requirement of certainty that is far from the 95%-99% certainty required of
cellphone science.
Finally, as to legal standards of proof, it is important to note that the
legal community has long recognized the fact that in special circumstances there
may be a need to create new standards. And one factor that has been used to
determine the need for such a revision is to ensure "the most desirable
result in terms of public policy".[23] Why
is there not such a rational option in the scientific community where lives are
potentially at stake?
19 - West's California Evidence Code, p. 72
20 - Ibid
21 - Black's Law Dictionary, p. 1381
22 - The citation in West's Annotated Codes,
Evidence Code, Sections 1-599, p. 561, 1995, sub-section 11 "Clear and
Convincing Evidence", and sub-section 12 "Absolute Certainty" make it clear that
even for this more stringent standard of proof it "does not mean absolute
certainty", "it is not equivalent to...unanswerable evidence", and "conclusive
proof is never necessary"
23 - West's California Evidence Code, p. 73
California Environmental Quality Act
The California Environmental Quality Act (CEQA) requires a "threshold of
significance" for a given detrimental environmental effect. Court cases have
interpreted this "threshold of significance" to be somewhere between a 10% and
30% chance of a detrimental environmental effect. So again, instead of requiring
a near certainty of environmental damage, as the scientific community demands,
the environmental community's threshold of 10%-30% can be expressed as requiring
only a standard of a "potential" for significant impact.
Some Thoughts on a New Standard For Cellphones
Why do we accept lower thresholds for the findings of legal liability and
environmental damage, than we accept for the acknowledgement of human health
hazards? One must query if this high standard is related to the economic needs
of industry rather than to sound public policy promoting human health. Whatever
the motives, it is essential that while we are reviewing the cellphone
literature we view these studies that are being published with a more sensible
standard, for the standard being applied verily militates against a finding of
harm—even when cases of harm exist.
Perhaps if there were a true intention to require such a high standard of
"scientific certainty" for cellphone research, it would be wise to also add into
the hopper for scientific "weight" the "risk factor" for cellphone use. Risk
factors (also known as "odds ratio", "OR", "relative risk", or "RR") are
routinely calculated to determine the correlation between the exposure to a
substance/activity and health effects. (See Figure 2, on the next page, which
compares cellphone risk to the known causes of cancer: asbestos, second-hand
tobacco smoke, X-rays, sunlight and radon, a naturally occurring radioactive
gas.).
For example, the risk factor for developing lung cancer from second-hand
smoke is 1.38 for highest exposed non-smokers. This risk factor translates to a
38% higher risk of developing lung cancer for those persons exposed to
second-hand smoke than for those persons who are not exposed to second-hand
smoke.
Because of this higher risk factor it makes sense that a correlation between
exposure to second-hand smoke and the risk of developing lung cancer is accepted
as common knowledge. But then it should also stand to reason that, with a risk
factor (using data from two of the studies discussed, below) of developing brain
tumors from cellphone exposure ranging from a factor of 3.9 to 8.19, the
connection between harm and cellphone use would also be accepted knowledge.
Amazingly, these risk factor numbers translate (for the youngest group for which
data has been collected in the Hardell and Hansson-Mild study) to a 719% higher
risk of developing a brain tumor from cellphone exposure than for those not
exposed to cellphones.
As we read each new cellphone study with ever-increasing evidence of harm,
but accompanied by contradictory conclusions of "no risk", it is important to
consider that our steadfast adherence to a scientific certainty standard
legitimizes a glaring public policy paradox. For, where cellphones are concerned,
the demand for "scientific certainty" is, in effect, a declaration that we are
more willing to condemn countless persons to carcinogenic exposure—risking death,
even for small children—than we are to condemn one person to death in a capital
criminal case with a sole requirement of "beyond a reasonable doubt".[24]
24 - Paraphrase from testimony given to the
California Public Utilities Commission by Dennis Zell, April 4, 2005, reported
by Cindy Sage
8. "No Known Mechanism" Argument
Perhaps the silliest "booby trap" is the argument that the data must be wrong
because "no known mechanism" exists to explain the results of the data. Stefan
Lönn, the lead author of Study 9, is one of many who have expressed this
sentiment. In reference to Study 9 he recently wrote, "... despite extensive
experimental research, there is still no known mechanism than can explain how
cell phone use might lead to an increased risk of cancer."[25]
Such an argument seems ever so reasonable. Without a known mechanism, how can
a finding be true? Yet, such an argument turns science on its head. The essence
of science is to examine experimental data. Yes, the data may have inaccuracies,
and, yes experiments need replication to provide confidence that some factor in
a given experiment may be unknowingly distorting the data, but given accurate
and replicated data, science has never required a known mechanism to validate
the data.
Such an argument is tantamount to denying that science is a valid process. It
is equivalent to saying, when the known mechanism for the cosmos was Newtonian
mechanics, that the precession of the orbit of Mercury around the sun "cannot
be". As we all know, a mechanism was eventually found: Einstein's theory of
relativity, which did provide a known mechanism for the long known scientific
truth that Mercury's orbital precession around the sun existed, albeit for some
time without a known mechanism.
9. The Role of "Industry"[26]
The final and most pervasive interpretive trap is, in
elephant-sitting-in-the-middle-of-the-room fashion, the trap that no one dare
speak its name: the role of the cellphone industry in cellphone science. And
yet, this trap is now almost the entire story when it comes to cellphone
research and any attempts to ensure safe use of cellphones.
To be fair, the cellphone industry, as one can well imagine, has much to be
concerned about if they did not become fully engaged in cellphone research: they
are concerned about manufacturing changes that they may be asked to make;
concerned about potential lawsuits (remember—the standard is lower for findings
of liability than for findings of health risk); and ultimately they are
concerned about a potential loss of the ever-increasing revenues of a $100
billion industry.[27]
25 - National Brain Tumor Foundation, Search,
Spring 2005, Issue No. 63
26 - Information in this section is a
compilation of information from Dr. Michael Kundi (Institute of Environmental
Health, Vienna, Austria), Dr. Louis Slesin (Publisher, Microwave News) and
attendance for 10 years to scientific meetings in the field
27 - The Wall Street Journal, Cellphone Makers
Say Radio Waves Are Found to Be Save, April 27, 2005, page D4
Despite the fact that the cellphone industry's intimate involvement with all
aspects of cellphone science poses a glaring conflict of interest, this industry
that has so many self-interest agendas, has managed to infiltrate all aspects of
cellphone science and now controls, to a large extent, the three major areas in
cellphone science: the funding, the science, and the creation of cellphone use
policy.
Funding
The cellphone industry pours large amounts of money into all aspects of this
field and consequently, controls the vast majority of the research through
funding their own studies, hiring key researchers from Universities and
government, sponsoring scientific meetings and putting financial pressure on
Universities and government to nix research in the field or fund only studies
designed to be incapable of finding a risk (e.g., using too few cases, or with
too short of a latency time). They also use their funding prowess for PR
obfuscation efforts that control the reports in the mass media (e.g., CBS is
owned by Viacom and cellphone advertising is a major source of media revenue).
Science
Not only does the cellphone industry control the research by being a major
source of funds of the research, it has also made sure that its personnel are
placed on safety standard committees and even into the World Health
Organization's EMF study projects. The cellphone industry also controls the
science by controlling the interpretation of the findings and how it is
presented - if it is presented?to the public. Again, as far as controlling
cellphone science interpretation, many of the cellphone corporations,
particularly Motorola, have their own "team" that travel the world attending all
the major meetings throughout the year, insisting on a negative "spin" of each
presentation that points to a finding of harm.
Policy
The cellphone industry controls the policy arm of cellphone science by
ensuring that all policy bodies (for example, the World Health Organization) are
led by or have a majority of cellphone industry personnel and consultants as
their staff. This is even true of scientific organizations: the
Bioelectromagnetics Society (BEMS), which ought to be comprised of independent
scientists instead is comprised of a large contingent of cellphone industry
employees or consultants including representatives from Motorola, Nokia,
Vodafone and the industry's trade association, the Cellular Telephone Industry
Association (CTIA). Not only are the science labs, governmental offices, and
science interest groups associated with cellphone science becoming operatives of
the phone industry, but the media?both in-house newsletters and public
journalism - is now so controlled by the phone industry that it is very
difficult to find a way to disseminate any other information about cellphone
exposure findings - other than the cellphone industry's official line of "no
harm".
The most chilling involvement of the cellphone industry is a growing campaign
to stop all cellphone research. At the 2003 BEMS meeting, Joe Elder, a Motorola
employee (formerly employed at the U.S. Environmental Protection Agency - EPA -
where he was in charge of the EPA's electromagnetic health review)[28], actually presented a slideshow—in the time slotted
for the sharing of scientific findings—that called for an expert panel of
"stakeholders" to call for, among other things, that cellphone studies be
"abort[ed]" based on the justification that "it is clear that there is nothing
to be found".[29]
28 - Elder's EPA report concluded:
"Biological effects occur at an SAR of about 1 W/kg; some of them may be
significant under certain environmental conditions."
29 - Mobile Telecommunications Health Research:
The Cancer Issue; Bioelectromagnetics Society meeting; June 27, 2003
A Reexamination of the Scientific Literature Regarding Cellphone Use
To complete the picture of the genuine risks that exposure to cellphones
presents, it is important to now reexamine the scientific literature as stand
alone evidence, as well as in the light of the interpretive traps discussed,
above.
To date there are now thousands of studies that examine the biological
consequences of exposures to electromagnetic fields and cellphones. These
studies range from cellular studies using various cell types from humans and
other animals (in-vitro studies) to live (in-vivo) animal studies - typically
rodents but other species, as well, including fruit flies. There now is also a
large body of epidemiological studies—the "in-vivo", if you will, study of human
populations focused on determining what, if any, biological responses large
human populations have experienced with exposure to cellphones.
For the purposes of this discussion on cellphone studies, the focus will be
limited to this epidemiological research. And because the epidemiological data
confirms that by far the strongest risk associated with cellphone exposure is
that of acoustic neuromas (tumors on the nerve from the ear to the brain), all
but one of the ten reports highlighted, below, will be reports that link
acoustic neuromas with cellphone exposure. The one exception will be the Hardell
& Hansson-Mild study (Study 10) that looks at aggregate brain tumor data by
age groups.
These studies show that from the beginning, rather than a cause for
sanguinity, there was cause to be concerned.
1. Board Presentation: CTIA, February 1999.
The earliest report that cellphone use increases the risk of acoustic
neuromas was made to the cellphone industry's own trade organization, the
Cellular Telecommunications Industry Association (CTIA). The person giving the
presentation, Dr. George Carlo,[30] was
probably as knowledgeable about the status of the research on the risk of brain
tumors from cellphone use as anyone on the planet. He ran the cellphone
industry's $25M research program for 6 years. The presentation he gave to the
full board of the CTIA was sobering. He told the board, "[T]he risk of acoustic
neuroma ... was 50 percent higher in people who reported cellphone use for six
years or more; moreover, that relationship between the amount of cellphone use
and this tumor appeared to follow a dose-response curve."[31]
2. NCI-Inskip, Cellular-Telephone Use and Brain Tumors, Dec. 2000
The earliest scientific report of a risk of acoustic neuromas as a result of
cellphone use came from a paper entitled "Cellular-Telephone Use and Brain
Tumors" conducted by a National Cancer Institute team.[32] The impetus for this study was a January 21, 1993 Larry King TV
interview of a grieving husband, David Raynard, who had just filed suit on
behalf of his wife Susan, alleging that her death from a brain tumor was the
result of cellphone use. Because of the public alarm raised by the Larry King
interview, the National Cancer Institute (NCI) pushed for an epidemiology study
of the risk of brain tumors from cellphone use. In spite of concerns that it was
too soon for a study because cellphones had not been in use long enough, the
study proceeded.
The resultant NCI-Inskip et al. study presented some 20 findings of the risk
of acoustic neuromas from cellphone use. Not one of the 20 results reached the
"statistical significance" level (i.e., >95% confidence, or equivalently a
p-value[33], the chance of no risk, <0.05).
The reason for the lack of statistical significance? There were too few cases of
acoustic neuromas.
This study included only 25 cases of acoustic neuromas (enrollment in the
study was between June 1994 and August 1998, a time when it was still rare for a
cellphone to be used).[34] Taken by itself,
this study gave little reason for concern, at least concern for short-term use
of a cellphone.
30 - Dr. George Carlo, Chief Scientist,
Wireless Technology Research (established by CTIA) oversaw multiple "scientific"
studies funded by the CTIA
31 - Page 205, Cell Phones, Invisible Hazards in
the Wireless Age by Dr. George Carlo and Martin Schram , Carroll & Graf
Publishers, Inc., 2001
32 - Inskip PD, et al.; Cellular-Telephone Use
and Brain Tumors. N Engl J Med 344(2):79-86, 2001
33 - p-value is a common term used by
statisticians. It refers to the probability of risk is due to chance. p-values,
in Table 1 and in all the following reports of p-values, are calculated from the
published 95% confidence intervals. Such calculations are subject to rounding
errors. Common practice is to term a p-value >0.05 as "statistically
significant". Subtracting 1 from the p-value given the "confidence", thus
p=0.05, or p=0.01, or p=0.001 is equivalent to 95%, 99% and 99.9% confidence.
WARNING: Calculated p-values are presented
throughout this discussion. Results with p-values larger than 0.05 require
caution and results with p-values larger than 0.10 require extreme caution. An
example of a severe misinterpretation result would be to interpret getting 3
heads on three consecutive tosses of a coin (p=0.125) means that there is a
12.5% chance that the coin is fair and a 87.5% chance that the coin is biased.
Such a result does imply that more coin tosses are required (equivalent to more
brain tumor cases) to determine if the coin is a fair coin.
34 - Mobile Telecommunications Health Research:
The Cancer Issue; Bioelectromagnetics Society meeting; June 27, 2003
However, the study did show for exposures of >3 years, or exposures
>100 hours, or use from 1993 or before, a possible increased risk of acoustic
neuromas, but with large p-values, it was hard to make a case for increased risk
(see the Warning in footnote 32). However, taken in the context of all the
studies that were to follow, this study was an early warning, that there
might be reason for concern. The odds ratios, with p-values, number of cases
and exposure time is summarized in Table 1 below:
Table 1, Risk of Acoustic Neuroma from Cellphone Use
Acoustic Neuroma[35] Odds Ratio[36] |
Calculated p-value |
Number of Acoustic Neuroma Cases |
Exposure Time |
1.4 |
0.438 |
10 |
> 3 years |
1.9 |
0.261 |
5 |
> 5 years |
1.4 |
0.445 |
9 |
> 100 cumulative hours |
1.0 |
NA |
1 |
> 500 cumulative hours |
1.2 |
0.733 |
6 |
Use began during 1992 or before |
1.3 |
0.764 |
2 |
Use began before 1990 |
3. Cellular Telephones and Cancer—a Nationwide Cohort Study in Denmark,
Feb 2001[37]
The next study, as was the case with the previous NCI-Inskip study, was
interpreted to put any concerns about the risk of brain tumors at bay.
35 - AN: Acoustic neuroma
36 - Odds Ratio: Odds ratios >1 indicates an
increased risk. Odds ratios <1 indicates a decreased risk. If there is no
risk, it can be expected that roughly 50% of the odds ratios will be >1 and
the other 50% will be <1
37 - Johansen et al., Journal of the National
Cancer Institute, Vol. 93, No. 3, February 7, 2001
Though this study did not report on acoustic neuromas directly it did include
a few cases of nerve sheath tumors. It is part of this discussion only because
nerve sheath tumors may include acoustic neuromas. This Danish cohort study was
published just a few weeks after the NCI-Inskip study, sponsored in part by two
large Danish cellular telephone operators, TeleDanmark and Sonofon. This study
reported, "... [No] association between use of these telephones and tumors of
the brain or salivary gland, leukemia, or other cancers." Because this study
relied on a cohort of 420,095 subscribers from 1982 to 1995, it purported to be
the definitive answer to the question of risk of cancers from the use of
cellphones. However, use of this cohort study, as a definitive answer was
illusory. As discussed above, cohort studies for rare diseases are quite limited.
This is true, not only because few cases can be expected for rare diseases, but
also because there were few cellphone users who would have used a cellphone long
nough to meet a cancer's expected latency time.
The Table 2 illustrates how few cellphone users were included in this study's
early years.
Table 2, Percent of Cellphone User in 3-Year Segments
Cellphone Users |
% Of Total |
Years |
Latency |
3,819 |
0.9 |
1982-1984 |
11 to 13 years |
7,866 |
1.9 |
1985-1987 |
8 to 10 years |
21,292 |
5.1 |
1988-1990 |
5 to 7 years |
96,959 |
23.1 |
1991-1993 |
2 to 4 years |
290,159 |
69.1 |
1994-1995 |
0 to 1 year |
The average use of analog cellphones in this study was only 3.5 years and
users of the digital cellphone averaged only 1.9 years. Yet this study is often
cited as "proof" that there should be no concern that cellphones cause cancer.
It is not surprising that this study with so few cellphone users in the early
years of the cohort and the extreme rarity of acoustic neuroma had almost
nothing to report. However, it did report 7 cases of nerve sheath tumor with
10.9 expected. Acoustic neuromas are a nerve sheath tumor, but it is unclear
whether any of these 7 cases were acoustic neuromas. If they were, based on the
US incidence data, there would only be 3, perhaps 4, acoustic neuromas; a number
too small to draw any conclusions.
4. Muscat, et al.; Handheld cellular telephones and risk of acoustic
neuroma, Apr. 2002[38]
This study also reported no risk of acoustic neuromas.
The abstract reports, "The relative risk was 0.9 (p = 0.07) and did not vary
significantly by the frequency, duration, and lifetime hours of use. Unless one
reads further, this study says there is no problem (remember, a result <1
indicates the possibility of a decreased risk).
However this acoustic neuroma study, funded by the cellphone industry,
found a non-significant increased risk of acoustic neuroma when the
cellphone was used for > 3 years (see Table 3, below).
Table 3, Risk of Acoustic Neuroma for Greater Than 3 Years of
Cellphone Use
AN Odds Ratio |
Calculated p-value |
Number of Patients |
Exposure Time |
1.7 |
0.361 |
12 |
> 3 Years |
Of the 90 acoustic neuroma patients in the study, only 18 patients (20%)
reported using a cellphone "regularly." The average cellphone use for patients
was 4.1 years. Yet for >3 years (12 patients), there was a 70% increased risk
of acoustic neuroma, albeit with such a large p-value, caution is important.
With only 18 patients using a cellphone "regularly" even the finding reported in
the abstract was not significant. Because there were only 12 patients using a
cellphone for greater than 3 years, the confidence level of the risk is further
diminished (i.e., the p-value was increased). It is disingenuous that the
abstract of this cellphone industry study reports a "non-significant"
decreased risk - without even a mention of a non-significant 70% increased
risk.
While this Muscat study reports that, "There was no evidence of a trend in
the odds ratio with increasing levels of exposure," the very scientist who
managed this study, Dr. George Carlo, reported that, "Muscat's analysis
concluded that the increased risk was statistically significant after patients
had used cell phones for six years."[39]
5. Cellular and cordless telephones and the risk for brain tumours, August
2002[40]
This study presents a strong reversal of what was reported in the previous 3
studies. And, as you will see in the studies that follow, the risk of acoustic
neuromas from cellphone use now becomes very clear.
38 - Muscat et al., Neurology 2002;58:1304-1306
39 - Dr. George Carlo and Martin Schram, Cell
Phones, Invisible Hazards in the Wireless Age, p170; Carrol & Graf
Publishers, Inc. 2001
40 - European Journal of Cancer Prevention:
Volume 11(4) August 2002 pp 377-386
A few months after the cellphone industry funded-Muscat study, an independent
group from Sweden's Örebro University and the National Institute for
Working Life (equivalent to the US OSHA) reported a strong risk of acoustic
neuromas with cellphone exposure.
It is important to recognize that this study's results probably
understated the risk of brain tumors because it excluded 37% of potential
cases. If the patient had died or was unable to fill out the questionnaire they
were excluded from the study. Also, all acoustic neuroma cases were
histologically confirmed. If there was only an MRI diagnoses of an acoustic
neuroma the case was excluded. As a result, it is likely that the histological
confirmation in this study severely underestimated the risk of acoustic neuromas
because radiation, rather than surgery, is common.
The median time for cellphone use to diagnosis of a brain tumor was 7, 5, or
3 years with analog, cordless and digital phones respectively. Table 4
summarized the risk of acoustic neuroma from cellphone exposure.
Table 4, Risk of Acoustic Neuroma by Phone Technology and
Cellphone Time of Use
AN Odds Ratio |
Calculated p-value |
Number of Cases |
Exposure Time |
Comments |
3.5 |
0.000163 |
38 |
< 1 Year |
Analog phones |
3.7 |
0.000298 |
26 |
> 5 Years |
3.5 |
0.115 |
7 |
> 10 Years |
1.03 |
0.910 |
30 |
< 1 Year |
Cordless phones |
1.8 |
0.232 |
11 |
> 5 Years |
2.0 |
0.556 |
2 |
> 10 Years |
1.2 |
0.524 |
23 |
< 1 Year |
Digital phones; Study period ended less
than 10 years after digital phones introduced |
2.0 |
0.556 |
2 |
> 5 Years |
|
|
0 |
> 10 Years |
With this study, for the first time there was evidence of an extraordinary
report of the risk of acoustic neuromas from exposure to cellphones. This
evidence should be viewed as especially compelling because the p-values were
very small, equivalent to an over 99.9% confidence level that the results were
not due to a chance finding.
Note also that all results for acoustic neuroma risks show an increased risk,
albeit often with a low confidence level. It is important to realize once again
that when there is a lack of confidence it is often the result of too few cases.
6. Further aspects on cellular and cordless telephones and brain tumours,
February 2003[41]
41 - Int J Oncol 2003 Feb;22(2):399-407
Six months later additional results from Study 5 were published. Not only had
the risk of acoustic neuromas increased to over 4-fold, the statistical
significance had also improved. Additionally there were now sufficient cases to
report results of the risk of tumors on the same side of the head where the
cellphone was used.
Not only was the risk increasing, but, for the first time, there was
sufficient data available to show trend data. This study found not only an
increased risk for acoustic neuroma, but also an increasing risk of acoustic
neuromas with each additional year of cellphone use. To wit: For every year of
cellphone use, the risk of acoustic neuroma increased by between 5% and 29% per
year, depending on the phone technology used (which, as was discussed in the
Interpretive Booby Traps Section, is relevant to use time, rather than to
inherent technology differences). Table 5 summarized the results.
Table 5, Risk of Acoustic Neuroma on Same Side of Head as
Cellphone Use and Risk Change Per Year by Phone Technology
AN Odds Ratio |
Calculated p-value |
Comments |
4.4 |
0.0000603 |
Analog phone |
4.2 |
0.00291 |
Analog phone, AN on same side as phone use |
1.4 |
0.203 |
Cordless phone |
1.3 |
0.437 |
Cordless phone, AN on same side as phone use |
1.4 |
0.203 |
Digital phone |
1.5 |
0.286 |
Digital phone, AN on same side as phone use |
AN Odds Ratio |
Calculated p-value |
Comments |
1.29 |
0.000718 |
Analog phones |
1.05 |
0.476 |
Cordless phones |
1.10 |
0.0934 |
Digital phones |
The results of this study are very similar to study 9, "Mobile Phone Use and
the Risk of Acoustic Neuroma, November 2004." Study 9 effectively replicates
this study's findings.
7. Vestibular schwannoma[42], tinnitus
and cellular telephones[43], March 2003
With previous studies now consistently pointing to a strong risk of acoustic
neuromas linked to cellphone exposure, researchers in this study turned their
focus to discerning if the incidence rate of acoustic neuromas exhibited a
change from pre-cellphone years to cellphone use years. To study if such a
change existed, the researchers examined the yearly change in the incidence rate
of acoustic neuroma for 2 time periods, 1960-1979 (before cellphones) and
1980-1998 (cellphone use begins in 1981).
42 - Vestibular schwannoma is another name
for acoustic neuroma
43 - Neuroepidemiology 2003 Mar-Apr;22(2):124-9
In the first period there was a small annual decrease in incidence
rate (-0.64% per year) of acoustic neuroma and in the second period there was a
large annual increase in the incidence rate (+2.16% per year).
Also examined, for the 2 time periods, was the annual change in the incidence
rate of "benign" brain tumors (excluding acoustic neuromas) and malignant brain
tumors. They found a shift in the annual change between the two time periods,
with the shift in the opposite direction. Acoustic neuromas went from
decreasing to increasing. "Benign" and malignant brain tumors went from
increasing to decreasing.
In the case of "benign" brain tumors, the 1960-1979 incidence rate change was
+2.38% per year, and the 1980-1998 incidence dropped sharply to nearly no annual
change (-0.01% per year). The results for malignant brain tumors were similar
but less pronounced: between 1960-1979 malignant brain tumors incidence rate
increased by +0.44% per year; by 1980-1998 it had decreased to -0.06% per year.
Once again, the insights found in this study are intriguing. Cellphones did
not exist in the early period but were introduced at the beginning (1981) of the
late period when the striking rise in acoustic neuromas also began. Some argue
that the change in the number of acoustic neuromas reflects, instead of the
introduction of cellphones, the introduction of better diagnostic tools;
specifically CT scans which began in 1974, and MRI scans, first used in 1984.
However, if this were true, then logically we would also see a similar change
(increased incident rate change) in "benign" and malignant brain tumors, but
instead we see the opposite: a decrease. Although not definitive, this study
adds to the "weight" of the evidence that there is indeed an adverse connection
between cellphones and harm. Figure 3, below shows the changes per year for the
3 tumor types.
This study also analyzed the risk of acoustic neuromas from cellphone use by
phone technology for various lengths of time. Again, we find a large increased
risk of acoustic neuromas. Again, we see the number of cases is highest for
analog phones, less for cordless phones, and lowest for digital phones,
consistent to the first Section's discussion on Technology Differences. Table 6
summarizes the results of this study.
Table 6, Risk of Acoustic Neuroma for Years of Cellphone Use
and Phone Technology
AN Odds Ratio |
Calculated p-value |
Number of Cases |
Time from first cellphone use |
Comments |
3.45 |
0.000219 |
38 |
> 1 Year |
Analog phones; median latency, 8 years |
3.71 |
0.00170 |
26 |
> 5 Years |
3.50 |
0.110 |
7 |
> 10 Years |
1.03 |
0.908 |
30 |
> 1 Year |
Cordless phones; median latency, 4 years;
risk increases with increasing time of exposure |
1.83 |
0.224 |
11 |
> 5 Years |
2.00 |
0.564 |
2 |
> 10 Years |
1.21 |
0.530 |
23 |
> 1 Year |
Digital phones; median latency, 3 years |
2.00 |
0.564 |
2 |
> 5 Years |
|
|
0 |
> 10 Years |
8. Christensen et al., Cellular Telephone Use and Risk of Acoustic
Neuroma[44], February 2004
This study does not show a risk of acoustic neuroma for longer durations of
cellphone use. Perhaps with only 2 cases using a cellphone for >10 years, the
lack of an increased risk for acoustic neuroma is the result of too little data?
44 - Christensen et al. Cellular Telephone
Use and Risk of Acoustic Neuroma, American Journal of Epidemiology; Vol. 159,
No. 3 (2004)
Further, not one of the reported results is "statistically significant",
making any determination of risk or lack of risk nearly meaningless. Yet the
study summarizes its findings, "The results of this ... study ... do not support
an association between cell phone use and risk of acoustic neuroma."
Once again, we see that the reason for "not statistically significant"
results is the lack of an inadequate number of cases: a total of 45 cases, and,
when examined for cellphone use of greater than a year, only 23 cases! Table 7
summarized the results.
Table 7, Risk of Acoustic Neuroma by Duration of Cellphone Use
AN Odds Ratio |
Calculated p-value |
Duration of Use |
Comments |
0.90 |
0.708 |
"Regularly" |
45 cases |
0.86 |
0.638 |
1-4 years |
23 cases |
0.68 |
0.305 |
> 5 years |
19 cases |
0.86 |
0.706 |
5-9 years |
17 cases |
0.22 |
0.0684 |
> 10 years |
2 cases |
1.58 |
0.520 |
Not reported |
Phone type not known, 5 cases |
0.26 |
0.0213 |
Not reported |
Analog cellphone, 4 cases |
1.11 |
0.733 |
Not reported |
Digital cellphone, 36 cases |
0.99 |
0.974 |
< 167.5 hours |
25 cases |
1.01 |
0.981 |
> 167.5 to 654 hours |
12 cases |
0.66 |
0.392 |
> 654 hours |
8 cases |
1.03 |
0.927 |
> 5 years but < 81.7 hours |
10 cases |
0.73 |
0.510 |
> 5 years and > 81.7 hours |
9 cases |
It is interesting to consider again that only 2 cases in this study used a
cellphone for 10 years or more as we juxtapose it with the next report (Study 9)
that found an acoustic neuroma risk with 12 cases of over 10 years of cellphone
exposure.
9. Mobile Phone Use and the Risk of Acoustic Neuroma[45], November 2004
In many ways this study can be seen as the tipping point between there may
be a risk to there is a risk. In part because it replicates Study 6 and in
part because its findings of risk for greater than 10 years of cellphone use are
so strong.
45 - Epidemiology 2004;15: 653-659
The study, partially funded by the cellphone industry, diagnosed acoustic
neuromas from 1999-2002. It has the longest time horizon yet. Because digital
cellphones were not introduced in Sweden until 1991, it is the first study to be
able to see a 10-year horizon clearly. Yet it reports, "Slightly less than 6% of
the population used mobile phones in 1990" that is, 10 years previously.
Nevertheless, its findings are alarming.
Unlike Study 4 above, Study 9 did not require histologically confirmed
diagnoses and, it also allowed radiological diagnoses (MRI or CAT scan). This
made a big difference in the numbers: thirty-nine percent of the cases were
diagnoses by histology, the remainder by radiological techniques.
The findings were as follows: "...[no indication of] an increased risk of
acoustic neuroma related to short-term mobile phone use after a short latency
period. However, our data suggest an increased risk of acoustic neuroma
associated with mobile phone use of at least 10 years' duration." [bold emphasis
is ours]
Yet an examination of the data (Table 8, below) indicates that even for
cellphone use of less than 10 years, there is a possible increased risk of
acoustic neuromas, albeit not a "statistically significant" risk. When the risk
of acoustic neuromas is examined by phone technology, we see an apparent higher
risk for analog phones used for 10 years or more, but roughly the same risk for
analog or digital phones for 5-9 years of >5 years of use. Table 9, below,
summarized these results.
The consistent theme that Study 9 illustrates is that the higher the duration
of use, the more the risk. This is true, not only for years of use, but also
for number of calls. As the number of hours increases, the odds ratio increases,
and then goes flat. However, the authors of Study 9 contradict their own
findings by reporting in their summary that the study, "found no association
between acoustic neuroma and amount of use measured as cumulative number of
hours or total number of calls."
Perhaps this is because the trend did not meet the threshold of 95%
confidence, but their reasons for their contradictory conclusion is never
explained.
Table 8, Cellphone Use and Risk of Acoustic Neuroma by Duration of Use
AN Odds Ratio |
Calculated p-value |
Cases |
Duration of Use |
Comments |
3.9 |
0.00224 |
12 |
> 10 years since first regular use |
Tumor on same side as phone use; Odds
ratios increase as the years of use increase. No trend was reported |
1.4 |
0.305 |
17 |
5-9 years since first regular use |
0.8 |
0.386 |
22 |
< 5 years since first regular use |
1.9 |
0.0904 |
14 |
> 10 years since first regular use |
Without regard to which side of the head
the phone was on; Odds ratios increase as the years of use increase. No
trend was reported |
1.1 |
0.729 |
30 |
5-9 years since first regular use |
0.8 |
0.350 |
44 |
< 5 years since first regular use |
AN Odds Ratio |
Calculated p-value |
Cases |
Cum No. of Calls |
Comments |
1.2 |
0.524 |
28 |
> 7350 calls |
Odds ratios increase as the number of
calls increase. No trend was reported. |
0.9 |
0.646 |
38 |
625 - 7349 calls |
0.8 |
0.476 |
18 |
< 625 calls |
AN Odds Ratio |
Calculated p-value |
Cases |
Cum Hrs of Use |
Comments |
1.1 |
0.739 |
21 |
> 450 hours |
Odds ratios increase, and then are flat,
as cumulative hours of use increase. |
1.1 |
0.667 |
44 |
30 - 449 hours |
0.7 |
0.194 |
17 |
< 30 hours |
Table 9, Cellphones Technology and Risk of Acoustic Neuroma
AN Odds Ratio |
Calculated p-value |
Cases |
Years of Use |
Comments, Types of Phones |
1.8 |
0.162 |
14 |
> 10 years since first "regular use" |
Analog phones (insufficient data for digital phones) |
1.3 |
0.505 |
13 |
5-9 years since first "regular use" |
Analog phones |
1.2 |
0.507 |
29 |
< 5 years since first "regular use" |
Digital phones |
10. Hardell and Hansson-Mild, Mobile and cordless telephones and the
association with brain tumours in different age groups[46]
This study shows that age at exposure matters, and for the young, it matters
a lot.
46 - Hardell and Hansson-Mild, in-press, Arch
Environ Health, click
here to view a PowerPoint presentation of this report and Arch Environmental
Health, vol 59, no 3 pp 132-7, march 2004 for the published version
The stunning data found in Study 10, has had the effect of breaking loose, if
ever so slightly, the many layers of denial surrounding the harmful health
effects of cellphone exposure. For the first time, not only have many in the
scientific community started expressing alarm, but as well, a number of public
media outlets as disparate as National Public Radio (November 2004) and O
Magazine (April 2005) have changed their "all good" attitude towards cellphones
to a "wait and see".
But the most significant import of Study 10 is that it links the risk of harm
to the age of the cellphone user. As discussed in the first Section on the
interpretive trap of "age at exposure", if cellphones are shown to be
tumorgenic, then when reading cellphone studies the presence of risk differences
in age groups is critical to an understanding of the degree of risk.
Looking at all forms of brain tumors, Hardell et al. found an 8-fold risk of
brain tumors in the youngest - the 20-29 year old age group. Further, this very
alarming finding is quite close to 95% confidence. When all ages are combined
there is a much smaller risk (1.31 fold), implying that the youngest carry by
far the largest proportion of the risk. For older age groups, only 50-59 years
old showed a near statistically significant risk of 1.56.
Of particular importance is to note that cordless phones (very similar to
digital cellphones, but in use for a longer period) also show a dramatically
higher risk for the youngest age group.
Table 10, illustrates the major findings of this study.
Table 10, Risk of Brain Tumor from Cellphone Exposure -
20-29 Age Group versus All ages from 5 years of cellphone use
Age Group |
Risk of BT[47] Odds Ratio |
Calculated p-value |
Phone Technology |
Cellphone Use (Years) |
20-29 |
8.17 |
0.0517 |
Analog |
5 |
All Ages |
1.31 |
0.0282 |
20-29 |
4.30 |
0.0201 |
Cordless |
All Ages |
1.40 |
0.0113 |
20-29 |
0.84[48] |
0.907 |
Digital |
All Ages |
1.11 |
0.571 |
47 - BT: Brain tumor
48 - Results based on one case and one control
As you can see, the 20-29 year age group is at greatest risk, except for
digital phones where there is insufficient data to draw any conclusion.
The significance of Study 10, thus, is that not only is there a connection of
harm to cellphone exposure, but that this harm creates a stronger risk for
younger brains, perhaps, as posited before, because they are still forming.
Study 10 is important to view within the context of the new cellphone
industry push to market cellphones to children. The CBS news report of this
industry's campaign, mentioned at the top of this discussion, is quite sobering
when the actual statistics are absorbed: "in 2003 one third of US kids aged
11-17 had their own cellphones; by the end of 2003, estimates grew to 40%. [By]
2004 nearly half of the kids in this age group had cellphones." This same show
went on to report/advertise-for-free that several toy makers were beginning
their marketing of specialized cellphones for kids 8-12.[49] And, that's not all: New campaigns by various corporations are
exploring an expansion of the cellphone market to preschoolers![50] Imagine what is in store for us when the cellphone
studies include exposure risks to these tender ages some many years hence.
49 - Good Morning America, Cellphones for
Kids, April 1, 2005
50 - "A Way to Calm a Fussy Baby: 'Sesame
Street' by Cell Phone", The Wall Street Journal, April 18, 2005
Some Sensible Recommendations
As we can see from the proceeding discussion, when the scientific literature
regarding human exposure to cellphones is interpreted within its own context,
which also includes the interpretive booby traps to be avoided, we have the
makings of a very serious health epidemic on our hands.
So what is to be done? Outright banning of cellphones at this juncture seems
both unwarranted and too extreme. However, a growing, worldwide community of
concerned scientists and health advocates are beginning to call for the
application of caution in the use of cellphones. This caution is recommended in
the form of invoking the "Precautionary Principle": the implementation of
low-cost mitigations - e.g., akin to the health warning labels that must now be
added to all tobacco advertising or packaging. As of 2003, the following
groupings have now invoked the Precautionary Principle with respect to the use
of mobile phones and the siting of mobile phone antennas:[51]
- Government of Italy (1998)
- Advisory Board on Non-Ionizing Radiation to the Czech National Institute
of Public Health (1999)
- Government of Switzerland (1999-2000)
- U.K. Independent Expert Group on Mobile Phones, also known as the
Stewart Committee (2000)
- Advisory Committee to the Director-General of Health of France, also
known as Zmirou Committee (2001)
- British Medical Association (2001)
- German Academy of Pediatrics (2001)
- German Radiation Protection Commission (2001)
- Advisory Panel to the Spanish Ministry of Health (2001)
- German Federal Office for Radiation Protection (2002)
- Russian National Committee on Non-Ionizing Radiation Protection (2002)
- City of Paris (2003)
- World Health Organization (WHO) International EMF Project (2003)
[Rescinded 3 months later after cellphone industry pressure was
applied.][52]
- City of San Francisco (2003)
Below are some simple, specific, and sensible recommendations to go along
with the invocation of the Precautionary Principle:
- New cellphone models should only be manufactured to include a headset
and without a speaker for the ear. The cost of this would be close to
zero, as it requires adding one item and deleting another.
- Cellphone owners with models made prior to implementation of
recommendation 1, need to be educated to only use their cellphone with
a headset.
- Children should be banned from using a cellphone. In the UK, this is
already a formal recommendation[53] as
it is for the Russian Academy of Sciences.[54]
- All cellphone owners need to be educated to carry the cellphone in the
"off" position to minimize their exposures. Thus functioning as an
answering machine, the cellphone can be periodically turned on for
accessing messages and returning calls.
51 - Microwave News, May/June 2003, p. 10
52 - "WHO Flip-Flops on EMFs, Precautionary
Principle Now Revoked", Microwave News, Vol. XXIII, No. 3, May/June 2003, page 1
53 - Associated Press report at Forbes
54 - Vladimir Binhi, Russian Academy of Sciences
In giving serious consideration to these low-cost responses to the potential
harm from cellphone use, it is important to remember two things. First, many
parallel recommendations have been implemented for tobacco use, despite the fact
that most of the scientific studies showing risk of tobacco exposure never
reached the high standards now imposed for cellphone exposure risk—and tobacco
becomes carcinogenic for probably no more than 10%-15% of the population.
Finally, it is also vital to contemplate that while we are busy enforcing an
impossibly high risk-of-harm bar, we are all engaged, with our now
near-universal cellphone use, in the world's largest human health experiment—an
experiment that has failed to provide us with the necessary informed consent.
All rights reserved per Sections 107 and 108 of the United States
Copyright Act. Permission is required to reproduce any portion or the entirety
of this publication in any form or by any means.
Requests: Lloyd Morgan (bilovsky (at) aol.com)
|